A new research study sheds light on a behavior that is consistent among many species — that is, making decisions based upon the actions of others.
Scientists at the University of Leeds believe they may have found why humans flock like sheep and birds, subconsciously following a minority of individuals.
Researchers discovered that it takes a minority of just five percent to influence a crowd’s direction — and that the other 95 percent follow without realizing it.
The findings could have major implications for directing the flow of large crowds, such as sporting events or public rallies or gatherings. The results may also be particularly useful in disaster scenarios where verbal communication may be difficult.
“There are many situations where this information could be used to good effect,” says Professor Jens Krause of the University’s Faculty of Biological Sciences.
“At one extreme, it could be used to inform emergency planning strategies and at the other, it could be useful in organising pedestrian flow in busy areas.”
Professor Krause, with PhD student John Dyer, conducted a series of experiments where groups of people were asked to walk randomly around a large hall. Within the group, a select few received more detailed information about where to walk. Participants were not allowed to communicate with one another but had to stay within arms length of another person.
The findings show that in all cases, the ‘informed individuals’ were followed by others in the crowd, forming a self-organizing, snake-like structure.
“We’ve all been in situations where we get swept along by the crowd,” says Professor Krause. “But what’s interesting about this research is that our participants ended up making a consensus decision despite the fact that they weren’t allowed to talk or gesture to one another. In most cases the participants didn’t realize they were being led by others.”
Other experiments in the study used groups of different sizes, with different ratios of ‘informed individuals’. The research findings show that as the number of people in a crowd increases, the number of informed individuals decreases. In large crowds of 200 or more, five per cent of the group is enough to influence the direction in which it travels.
The research also looked at different scenarios for the location of the ‘informed individuals’ to determine whether where they were located had a bearing on the time it took for the crowd to follow.
“We initially started looking at consensus decision making in humans because we were interested in animal migration, particularly birds, where it can be difficult to identify the leaders of a flock,” says Professor Krause. “But it just goes to show that there are strong parallels between animal grouping behavior and human crowds.”
The paper relating to this research, entitled Consensus decision making in human crowds is published in the current issue of Animal Behavior Journal.
A related study conducted in 2013 examined herd mentality in online communities. The researchers (Taylor et al., 2013) examined comments they manipulated on a single website with up and down votes. If a comment had been given a fake up-vote, the first person reading the comment would add an additional up-vote to the comment. This effect only translated to up-votes, not down-votes, however.
This more recent study suggests that ‘herd mentality’ also operates online and in online communities. People appear to be subconsciously swayed by the opinion of others.
Because both of these studies were relatively small, it’s not clear how robust these findings are. Additional research will need to be conducted to confirm these studies’ findings, and to more closely examine what other psychological or personality factors may help explain why some people go along with the crowd, while others do not.
Source: University of Leeds